Quantcast

Study: Bilingual infants lip-​​read more than monolingual infants

New research from North­eastern devel­op­mental psy­chol­o­gist David J. Lewkowicz shows that infants learning more than one lan­guage do more lip-​​reading than infants learning a single language.

In the study, bilin­gual and mono­lin­gual infants were observed watching a video of a woman speaking in Spanish or Catalan; the infants were learning one or both of these lan­guages. Lewkowicz and his col­lab­o­ra­tors found that bilin­gual infants focused their atten­tion on the mouth at an ear­lier age and for a longer period of time than mono­lin­gual infants. This sug­gests that bilin­gual infants pick up on salient audio­vi­sual speech cues more than their mono­lin­gual peers to help them dis­tin­guish between the two lan­guages they are learning simul­ta­ne­ously, he said.

These results pro­vide new insights into the under­lying mech­a­nisms of people’s ability to acquire more than one lan­guage at the same time early in life,” said Lewkowicz, a pro­fessor in the Depart­ment of Com­mu­ni­ca­tion Sci­ences and Dis­or­ders in Northeastern’s Bouvé Col­lege of Health Sci­ences.

Lewkowicz and his co-authors—Ferran Pons and Laura Bosch, both at the Uni­ver­sity of Barcelona and the Insti­tute for Brain, Cog­ni­tion, and Behavior in Spain—report these find­ings in a forth­coming paper in the journal Psy­cho­log­ical Sci­ence. Lewkowicz said the find­ings have impor­tant impli­ca­tions for under­standing how infants acquire speech and lan­guage and shed light on how bilin­gual infants—despite their neural and behav­ioral immaturity—manage to learn two dif­ferent lan­guages as easily as mono­lin­gual infants learn one language.

The find­ings, he said, could also play a role in treating and diag­nosing chil­dren with com­mu­nica­tive and learning dis­or­ders like autism. He noted that chil­dren with autism are typ­i­cally diag­nosed between 18 and 24 months and tend to look at faces less often than typ­ical chil­dren, a factor that sig­nif­i­cantly reduces their oppor­tu­ni­ties for expe­ri­encing audio­vi­sual speech that Lewkowicz’s pre­vious research has shown to be impor­tant for young babies. What this means in the con­text of his research is that iden­ti­fying infants with par­tic­ular pat­terns of selec­tive atten­tion to speech could help diag­nose com­mu­ni­ca­tion and learning dis­or­ders ear­lier than is cur­rently possible.

In this new study, Lewkowicz and his col­lab­o­ra­tors used eye-​​tracking tech­nology to mea­sure pre­cisely how much time infants attended to the eyes and mouth—the two crit­ical areas that we focus on during social communication—of a person who could be seen and heard speaking. The infants all lived in Spain and were learning Spanish and/​or Catalan.

The researchers found that, regard­less of what lan­guage the person spoke, 4-​​month-​​old mono­lin­gual babies looked longer at the eyes than the mouth but that 4-​​month-​​old bilin­gual babies looked equally as long at the eyes and mouth. Sim­i­larly, whereas 12-​​month-​​old mono­lin­gual babies looked equally at the eyes and mouth in response to native speech and more at the mouth in response to non-​​native speech, 12-​​month-​​old bilin­gual babies looked longer at the mouth regard­less of lan­guage and, crit­i­cally, longer than mono­lin­gual infants in both cases.

These find­ings build upon Lewkowicz’s ground­breaking research pub­lished in 2012, which showed that babies learn to talk not only by lis­tening to sounds but also by lip-​​reading. Find­ings showed that as babies start to babble—at around six months of age—they begin to shift their atten­tion to the mouth of the person speaking. In essence, they begin to lip-​​read, having dis­cov­ered how much salient speech infor­ma­tion they can learn from watching a person’s mouth.

In the pre­vious study, two exper­i­ments were con­ducted with dif­ferent age groups of mono­lin­gual, English-​​learning infants between 4 and 12 months of age. In these exper­i­ments, the infants watched videos of women speaking either Eng­lish or Spanish. In addi­tion to finding that babies begin to shift their atten­tion to a talker’s mouth after 6 months of age, this study showed that 12-​​month-​​olds attended to the mouth longer when the person spoke Spanish than when she spoke Eng­lish, pre­sum­ably because what had now become an unfa­miliar lan­guage was more dif­fi­cult to understand.

These find­ings led the researchers to pursue this latest study. Upon learning that com­bined audi­tory and visual speech infor­ma­tion is very impor­tant for speech and lan­guage acqui­si­tion in infancy, they won­dered whether babies growing up in a bilin­gual envi­ron­ment take even greater advan­tage of these com­bined audi­tory and visual cues and, thus, whether they lip-​​read even more.

That’s pre­cisely what we found,” Lewkowicz said, adding that “These find­ings shed new light on bilin­gualism, which hap­pens to be of great interest to researchers studying  the effects of early expe­ri­ence as well as pol­i­cy­makers con­cerned with inte­grating non-​​English speaking chil­dren into the classroom.”

Lewkowicz noted that his team’s research offers insight into what kinds of infor­ma­tion to expose chil­dren to in order to help them acquire two lan­guages more effec­tively. Babies, he explained, go through an intense period of learning in the first year and during this time they acquire exper­tise in their native lan­guage. Para­dox­i­cally, while native-​​language exper­tise emerges, babies’ ability to per­ceive other lan­guages declines—a process known as per­cep­tual nar­rowing, he said.

By the end of their first year, infants have nar­rowed their ability to per­ceive dif­ferent lan­guages if they have not had expo­sure to them,” he explained. “This process hap­pens very rapidly and ear­lier than we thought, though chil­dren still retain a great deal of plas­ticity throughout child­hood. But cer­tainly, if you don’t get exposed to another lan­guage during child­hood, you’ll have dif­fi­culty learning it later on.

The flip side of this is that expo­sure to mul­tiple lan­guages in infancy pre­vents nar­rowing, and it is now clear that one way in which bilin­gual infants manage to accom­plish their task is by taking max­imum advan­tage of both audible and vis­ible speech when­ever they interact with their social partners.” Original press release.




The material in this press release comes from the originating research organization. Content may be edited for style and length. Want more? Sign up for our daily email.