New! Sign up for our email newsletter on Substack.

Study explores infant body position and learning

A developmental psychologist at the University of California, Riverside, has completed a study that is the first to measure how often infants spend time in different body positions over the first year of life.

Theย study, published in the journal Infancy, aims to understand how the physical context of infantsโ€™ everyday experiences โ€“ in particular, how much time they spend in different body positions โ€“ changes over the course of the first year and how these changes are predicted by infantsโ€™ developing motor skills.

โ€œI was surprised to find that 3-month-olds are held almost half of their waking daysโ€ saidย John Franchak, an assistant professor ofย psychology, who performed the study. โ€œTwelve-month-olds are held much less frequently and spend most of their time on the ground. How often infants play, crawl, walk, or sit changes how they interact with objects and changes, too, the physical way in which they interact with other people.โ€

Specifically, Franchak found that sitting, upright, and prone (belly towards the ground regardless of contact with the ground) accounted for less thanย 7 percent of the 3-month-old infantโ€™s day. By 12 months, these positions accounted for 62 percent of the infantโ€™s day.

The cross-sectional study sought to understand what a 12-month-old learns and how this learning differs from that of, say, a 3-month-old infant, based on their everyday experiences. Toward that end, the study tested separate groups of 3-month-olds, 6-month-olds, 9-month-olds, and babies that were a year old. It used data acquired for 95 babies from across the United States.

The study used an innovative approach to acquire this data: the infantsโ€™ caregivers were sent text messages five times a day for a week to inquire on what the baby was doing at that moment. The caregivers electronically reported infant body positions immediately thereafter in brief one-minute surveys. They also reported infantsโ€™ location โ€“ whether the infants were on the floor or up on a raised surface โ€“ and the onset of sitting, crawling, and walking. The study is the first to use this method, called ecological momentary assessment, to measure infantsโ€™ behaviors.

โ€œWe have plenty of data on what babies do in the lab, where we measure their development by doing some assigned task,โ€ Franchak said. โ€œWhat we donโ€™t know is what drives that development, what happens in the days, hours, and minutes they are at home, where they experience a number of things that lets them learn. Until this study, we didnโ€™t know how often babies sit, crawl, and stand in everyday life, outside the lab. The ecological momentary assessment the study used offers a better sense of infantsโ€™ actual lives versus a slice of life in the lab, and gives a more realistic distribution of their different types of body positions and experiences. Understanding these differences allows us to build better theories about how infants develop and learn from the world.โ€

Franchak explained that body positions change dramatically over the first year of life, and much of that change results from infants acquiring new motor skills.ย  Typically, babies begin to sit aroundย 6 months of age, crawl at aroundย 8 months, stand aroundย 11 months, and walk when they are a year old. It is important to study infant body position, he said, because changes in how babies interact with the world change their opportunities for learning. Learning to sit is linked with better object perception. Learning to walk is linked with improved language ability.

โ€œThe amount of time infants spend in different positions shapes their visual and manual activity โ€“ impacting perceptual, cognitive, and social development โ€“ and reflects opportunities to practice and develop motor skills,โ€ Franchak said. โ€œFor example, infants rarely see faces while playing on the ground in sitting, upright, and prone positions, but see faces more often when held or sitting off the ground as in a high chair.

โ€œFurther, infants who can sit independently atย 6 months spend more time sitting in daily life.ย  This allows them to manipulate objects more frequently and receive nearly twice as much opportunity to experience the richer visual-manual exploration of objects than prone or supine infants,โ€ he added. โ€œLearning to walk changes social interactions with caregivers and predicts improvement in infantsโ€™ spatial cognition.โ€

Franchak cautioned that caregivers play an important role in influencing the studyโ€™s results.ย  The studyโ€™s youngest infants are entirely dependent on caregivers to change their body position. For older infants, choosing to sit or stand is often an option only if their caregivers provide the opportunity.

โ€œI canโ€™t say enough about how much caregivers are doing in determining what positions their babies are in,โ€ he said. โ€œCaregivers act in response to how they perceive their children. When they see their babies have acquired certain skills, they take action accordingly to accommodate these skills. Caregivers and their babies then negotiate constantly on how best to proceed thereafter.โ€

Theย study, titled โ€œChanging opportunities for learning in everyday life: Infant body position over the first year,โ€ was supported by UCR through a Regents Faculty Fellowship to Franchak.

There's no paywall here

If our reporting has informed or inspired you, please consider making a donation. Every contribution, no matter the size, empowers us to continue delivering accurate, engaging, and trustworthy science and medical news. Independent journalism requires time, effort, and resourcesโ€”your support ensures we can keep uncovering the stories that matter most to you.

Join us in making knowledge accessible and impactful. Thank you for standing with us!



Leave a Comment

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.