Early spatial reasoning predicts later creativity and innovation, especially in STEM fields

Exceptional spatial ability at age 13 predicts creative and scholarly achievements over 30 years later, according to results from a new longitudinal study published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

The study, conducted by psychology researcher David Lubinski and colleagues at Vanderbilt University, provides evidence that early spatial ability — the skill required to mentally manipulate 2D and 3D objects — predicts the development of new knowledge, and especially innovation in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) domains, above and beyond more traditional measures of mathematical and verbal ability.

“We live in the age of human capital,” says Lubinski. “Creativity is the currency of the modern era, especially in STEM disciplines. Having a better understanding of the human attributes that facilitate innovation has clear practical implications for education, training, business, and talent development.”

And yet, despite longstanding speculation that spatial ability may play an important role in supporting creative thinking and innovation, there are very few systems in place to track skill in spatial reasoning:

“Current procedures for identifying intellectually precocious youth currently miss about half of the top 1% in spatial ability,” Lubinski explains.

Using data from a study that began in the late 1970s, Lubinski and colleagues followed up with 563 students who had scored exceptionally well — in the top 0.5% — on the SATs at age 13. The researchers also examined data on the participants’ spatial ability at age 13, as measured by the Differential Aptitude Test.

300px-Eakins,_Baby_at_Play_1876Confirming previous research, the data revealed that participants’ mathematical and verbal reasoning scores on the SAT at age 13 predicted their scholarly publications and patents 30 years later.

But spatial ability at 13 yielded additional predictive power, suggesting that early spatial ability contributes in a unique way to later creative and scholarly outcomes, especially in STEM domains.

Importantly, these results confirm longstanding speculation in the psychological sciences that spatial ability offers something important to the understanding of creativity that traditional measures of cognitive abilities used in educational and occupational selection don’t capture.

Lubinski believes cultivating these skills is imperative for ensuring scientific innovation.

“These students have exceptional and under-challenged potential, especially for engineering and technology,” Lubinski explains. “We could do a much better job of identifying these students and affording them better opportunities for developing their talents.”


Substack subscription form sign up

1 thought on “Early spatial reasoning predicts later creativity and innovation, especially in STEM fields”

  1. That’s interesting. It’s been also suspected and some have shown good evidence that spatial relationship are about doubled if a child is taught how to play a musical instrument well. Because visualization was important for Einstein to understand and create his Relativity, it makes sense. He played the violin, also. Because Darwin’s finches, helped him see how evolution could occur, and the same ideas came to Wallace when he was studying the types and kinds of species on the Indonesian isles years later, it makes sense. Darwin was no musician, however he went on a 5 years long field trip on the Beagle and that could have had some influence on his spatial relationships abilities, too.

    As a student I’d been on lots of field trips before I went to college. & in college seemed to do very, very well on field trips. I’ve developed a model of creativity and how it relates to such tasks, but there’s no room to write much here.

Comments are closed.